Studying the psychological impact of exercise deprivation: are experimental studies hopeless?
Szabo, Attila
Understanding the physical and psychological consequences of
deprivation from regular exercise, in committed exercisers, is an
important issue because today exercise is a not only a personal but also
a social striving. Researchers who venture in this field of study may
become quickly discouraged when they realize how limited is the
literature in this area of research. Citation indexes and well compiled
computerized data bases are virtually void of studies on the effects of
exercise deprivation. What may be the reason for the shortage of work in
this field? The answer is simple: People exercise for some sort of
benefit(s). No matter what that benefit may be, from a personal
perspective it is most often greater than the benefit derived from
participation in an exercise-deprivation study. Therefore, highly
devoted exercisers are unlikely to enroll in deprivation studies even if
there were some alluring incentives involved (Baekeland, 1970).
To date, there are less than ten experimental studies that examined
the psychological impact of exercise deprivation and only a little more
than ten opportunistic or survey-type studies (Szabo, 1995). But even
more disappointing is the fact that most of these studies looked to the
problem of exercise deprivation indirectly. More precisely, they
examined the concept in relation to "exercise addiction" based
on the presumption that negative emotions during episodes of exercise
deprivation are reflections of addiction to exercise (e.g., Anshel,
1991). Accordingly, the primary objective in these studies was not the
understanding of how or what people feel during intervals of exercise
deprivation, but rather whether they are or are not addicted to their
exercise (e.g., Anshel, 1991; Gauvin, 1990; Sachs & Pargman, 1979).
Therefore, the examination of the effects of exercise deprivation was
most often secondary to the analysis of issues related to exercise
addiction. The possibility of isolating exercise deprivation from the
concept of addiction was stressed only recently (Szabo, 1995; Wingate,
1993). This isolation is necessary in the conceptual foundation of
experimental inquiries aimed to the understanding of how regular (not
addicted) exercisers feel when they are deprived of their exercise. Once
this foundation is established, the realization of experimental studies
may present major difficulties.
The Subject-Recruitment Dilemma
As noted above, few people volunteer for exercise deprivation
studies. Those who are willing to take part in these studies may be
different in some way from those who do not take part in similar
investigations for any incentive (Baekeland, 1970). How different? In
fact volunteers for exercise deprivation inquiries may represent a
totally different population than the population from which they are
thought to be. A sturdy example for this assertion stems from a recent
experience. Recruitment leaflets calling for participation in an
exercise deprivation study were distributed to more than 5000
participants in a large marathon (Gauvin & Szabo, 1996). Only 24
people ([less than]0.5%!) showed interest in participation by replying
to the call. In terms of probability, as based on this observation, it
may be estimated that less than five marathon runners in a thousand may
be interested in volunteering for exercise deprivation studies. But who
are those five people? How are they different from the others? Do they
possess some unique personality characteristics? Or it is their
relationship with their exercise that is unique? Regrettably no answers
are available to these questions at this time. One can speculate and
hypothesize, but only the empirical testing of these hypotheses will
shed light on the questioned differences. While it may be argued that
marathoners do not represent the general exercising population and that
the above figure may be different in subjects committed to other forms
of exercise, the key point that merits attention is commitment. Other
exercisers may be as committed to their form of exercise as runners
(Gauvin & Szabo, 1992), but they were not studied as intensively as
runners (Szabo, 1995). When deprived from their favorite activity, all
committed exercisers may experience hardship (Anshel, 1991; Baekeland,
1970; Crossman, Jamieson, & Henderson, 1987; Gauvin, 1990; Gauvin
& Szabo, 1992; Wingate, 1993). Therefore, the unwillingness to take
part in deprivation studies, observed in runners, may be comparable to
the general "exercise-committed" population at least until
more exercise-specific data will be available.
To elucidate further on why people are reluctant to take part in
exercise deprivation studies, the attitudes of a runners' group
were sought on the information highway. A message, asking runners
whether or not they would take part in an exercise deprivation study,
was posted on the Internet news-group "rec.running". (The
procedure was not intended to represent a "scientific"
inquiry, but rather to probe into runners' viewpoint and to use
their opinion as an aid in the current discussion.) A brief summary of
the answers given by the 10 runners who replied to the message is
presented in Table 1. Clearly, none of the runners expressed
unconditional will for taking part in a deprivation study. One vivid
reason for this resistance was phrased by one of the runners: "Life
is too short not to do the things you like to do. . ."(1)
Incentives for Participation
Incentives in deprivation studies may play a role. Some people may be
attracted with some incentives, especially monetary offers. However, the
experimenter(s) goes against the value of basic needs when such
incentives are offered. A "hooked" runner (Lewis, 1984;
Mickel, 1979) or exerciser may be compared to the "hooked"
smoker. These people may be allured by certain incentives and believe
that they can comply with the requirements of a behavioral deprivation
protocol, but once started their willpower may be shattered by the
strong craving for the behavior to which they are hooked. At that point
the researcher may be faced with the chance of noncompliance and, unless
there are some control measures, the reliability of the data may be
shattered. To assign weight to this dilemma a statement made by one of
the runners ought to be reproduced. "If by chance, I did say yes,
(to participation in a deprivation study) it would be very difficult not
to cheat."
It may be argued that people who are not offered an incentive for
participation may also become non-compliant. However, these people have
almost nothing (i.e., an important incentive) to lose if they are not
conforming to the requirements of an experimental protocol. Therefore,
they may be less likely to become dishonest, because the incentive-based
temptation is not present. Monetary incentives are not as critical as
incentives that may bound more strongly, such as grade points offered to
university students (e.g., Gauvin & Szabo, 1992) in exchange for
their participation. Such incentives may become crucial for some people
and assume the a role of a basic need as the behavior for which they are
studied. When two basic needs are in conflict the subjects'
compliance is extremely unpredictable which shatters the
data-reliability.
The Length of Deprivation
As noted in Table 1, apart from monetary reward, the length or the
duration of the deprivation period was another condition stated by two
runners who showed interest in participating in an exercise deprivation
study. Beyond doubt, the longer the deprivation period the harder may be
the adherence to the requirements of the inquiry. However, to estimate
the consequences of exercise deprivation in real life settings, the
deprivation period in experimental studies should mimic the naturally
occurring deprivation periods. Clearly, short periods of deprivation,
lasting one (Thaxton, 1882) or two days (Tooman, Harris, & Mutrie,
1985), may coincide either with a planned or an opportunistically
assimilated rest period. An example for this assertion stems from an
inquiry by Crossman et al. (1987). In this study the authors concluded
that exercise deprivation had no negative effect on teenagers who had a
break from running for one day or from swimming for five days. However,
the subjects knew in advance about their training suspension. When the
interruption is planned it can no longer be classified as exercise
deprivation. Quoting another runner may illustrate this point: "I
often schedule time off from running and experience little discomfort
from that time off. In fact, it puts running into perspective and keeps
other aspects of my life healthier". Therefore, short-duration
inquiries have limited value in exercise deprivation literature because
they are biased by diurnal variations in mood/affect (Clark, Watson,
& Leeka, 1989), planned time-off, and by the arising opportunities
to fulfil other obligations or to participate in other activities. But
the status of exercise deprivation literature is very deficient if
short-duration studies are overlooked. To date, only three studies have
adopted an exercise deprivation period that lasted one week or longer
(Baekeland, 1970; Gauvin & Szabo, 1992; Morris, Steinberg, Sykes
& Salmon, 1990). While it is clear that there is a need for similar
inquires, their realization may be hopeless.
Table 1.
Summary of Runners, Feelings About Participation in an
Exercise-deprivation Study.
Runner Gender Willingness
to participate
1 male definitely not
2 female most likely no
3 male yes, depending on the
length of deprivation
4 male yes, depending on the
length of deprivation
5 male yes, depending on the
season of the year
6 female yes, motivated by
monetary incentive
7 male yes, depending on the
season of the year
8 male probably not
9 female not even for money
10 female yes, for monetary
incentive
Note: The runners were asked whether they would be willing to take
part in an exercise deprivation study.
The Season
In some forms of exercise the season plays an important role. Running
is a good example. Season was the condition put forward by two
interrogated runners. It goes without explanation that it is easier to
commit oneself to a training interruption when the season does not favor
an adopted activity. However, the season, chiefly in case of runners, is
not only tied to weather but also to the time of public races. In
northern regions running races are less frequent in the winter than in
the summer. Some people may even take a couple of weeks or months of
break from their exercise when both weather and lack of opportunity for
challenge decline. These people could become candidates for exercise
deprivation studies in that season. However, their participation may be
biased by the previously discussed notion of planned versus unplanned
training-interruption. Opportunistic studies may take advantage of
planned exercise layoff, but these inquiries cannot answer key questions
regarding the effects of coercive exercise deprivation.
Mission Impossible?
lt is obvious that recruitment of subjects for experimental studies
is a difficult problem. Further, volunteers are most likely different
from the non-volunteers for reasons that remain to be tested. Moreover,
the difficulties are topped by the fact that the offering of incentives
for participation and/or the acceptance of certain conditions put
forward by the subjects can distort the answer to the posed question.
Currently, there are three possible solutions that may be thought to
solve the problem of reliability. However, two of these solutions harden further the recruitment of subjects and one of them may be answering a
different question.
The first possible solution for gathering reliable data in exercise
deprivation studies is the direct observation of subjects in an isolated
environment. However, this method gives rise to a new confound which is
the impact of isolation. In institutional settings (i.e., prisons, army,
readaptation centers, etc.), where people are habituated to confinement,
an overt control for exercise deprivation is possible, but people in
these settings do not always represent the general population (Szabo,
1995). Therefore, direct observation strategy remains a theoretical,
rather than practical, solution in the area of exercise deprivation.
The second conceivable solution is the employment of Caltrac
equipment that gauges caloric expenditure by vertical displacement. The
monitoring of caloric expenditure could also control for the
substitution of one activity with another, that may be a confound in
deprivation studies. However, the obstacle is that the use of such
apparatus is costly and these instruments are inefficient in certain
activities, such as stationary bicycling, weight lifting, and other
types of exercises without vertical displacement. Thus, the use of such
device has some but not utter value in assuring the reliability of the
experimental data.
Finally, a third possible solution is the granting of certain
flexibility (as opposed to rigidity) for subjects with regard to the
adherence to the protocol (e.g., Morris et al., 1990; Gauvin &
Szabo, 1992). Accordingly, subjects are told that they should try
(instead of must) to stop exercising, but if they have an urge for
exercising they may do so without negative consequences. In these
conditions the subjects record their exercise so that the volume of the
actual deprivation can be contrasted with the target volume. Such a
"flexible" approach may generate a less coercive atmosphere
and also stimulate subjects, adherence, but it will most likely result
in the gauging of a reduction in training as opposed to exercise
deprivation. Therefore, this "soft" method may be useful to
some extent, but it is not the optimal method for answering the question
whether forced or involuntary exercise deprivation has an impact on the
exercising individual.
It appears that thorough control in experimental studies addressing
the issue of exercise deprivation may be futile. This is not to say that
experimental studies seem to be useless, but simply to emphasize that
most control measures may not resolve the problem of subject-related
confounds. These confounds should be recognized and dealt with in the
most appropriate manner but their elimination cannot be conceived at
this time. Therefore, future experimental studies should be developed in
such a way that these confounds are accounted for in advance and
highlighted in the data interpretation of the results. Perhaps one of
the most puzzling problems, begging for scrutiny, is the description of
the participants, in contrast to nonparticipants, in exercise
deprivation studies.
The Extra Weight of Descriptive and Opportunistic Research
Descriptive data may not yield accurate results, because of
distortions in memory and lack of proper comparison standards, but they
are certainly helpful in dealing with the key problem: subject
recruitment. Although they are often appraised as being limited in
value, descriptive inquiries are and will be important resources of
knowledge in exercise deprivation studies. Their utility should not be
undermined in this particular field of study. However, carefully
formulated opportunistic research may be even more helpful. Chan and
Grossman (1988) examined exercise deprivation by using such an approach.
These authors studied a group of injured runners whose injuries did not
interfere with any other routine activity than running. The latter is of
paramount importance in the separation of the observed effects due to
injury and due to deprivation from exercise. Similar type of
opportunistic studies may be carried out more easily than experimental
studies in future. In this specific field of study, in which the
performance of experimental research is almost impossible, the value of
descriptive and opportunistic research merits some extra weight.
Conclusion
To date, the psychological effect of exercise deprivation has been
studied sparsely. The difficulty in subject recruitment is the main
cause for the lack of experimental inquiries. Further, participants are
likely different, in yet unknown way(s), from nonparticipant and
consequently the results of the experimental studies cannot be
generalized. Existing control measures are weak for the elimination of
confounds in experimental studies. While it sounds pessimistic,
non-experimental studies may be the main sources of knowledge for
psychologists interested in the effects of exercise deprivation.
Footnote
1 All the quotations are reproduced with permission from runners.
References
Anshel, M. H. (1991). A psycho-behavioral analysis of addicted versus
non-addicted male and female exercisers. Journal of Sport Behavior, 14,
145-154.
Baekeland, F. (1970). Exercise deprivation: Sleep and psychological
reactions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 22, 365-369.
Chan, C. S., & Grossman, H. Y. (1988). Psychological effects of
running loss on consistent runners. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66,
875-883.
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Leeka, J. (1989). Diurnal variations
in the positive affect. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 205-234.
Crossman, J., Jamieson, J., & Henderson, L. (1987). Responses of
competitive athletes to lay-offs in training: Exercise addiction or
psychological relief? Journal of Sport Behavior, 10, 28-38.
Gauvin, L. (1990). An experiential perspective on the motivational
features of exercise and lifestyle. Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences,
15, 51-58.
Gauvin, L. & Szabo, A. (1992). Application of the experience
sampling method to the study of the effects of exercise withdrawal on
well-being. Journal of Sport Exercise Psychology, 14, 361-374.
Gauvin, L. & Szabo, A. (1996). The impact of deprivation from
running on the psychological states of marathoners: A daily monitoring
study. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Lewis, R. (1984, January). Some athletes let their "fix"
take precedence over everything else. Women's Sports, 14-17.
Mickel, H. (1979, April) The true runner becomes hooked on a feeling.
Runner's World, 30-31.
Morris, M., Steinberg, H., Sykes, B. A., & Salmon, P. (1990).
Effects of temporary withdrawal from regular running. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 34, 493-500.
Sachs, M. L., & Pargman, D. (1979). Running addiction: A depth
interview examination. Journal of Sport Behavior, 2, 143-155.
Szabo, A. (1995). The impact of exercise deprivation on well-being of
habitual exercisers. The Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in
Sports, 27(3). 68-75.
Thaxton, L. (1982). Physiological and psychological effects of
short-term exercise addiction on habitual runners. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 4, 73-80.
Tooman, M. E., Harris, D. V., & Mutrie, N. (1985). The effect of
running and its deprivation on muscle tension, mood and anxiety. Paper
presented at the congress of the International Society for sports
Psychology, Copenhagen, Denmark.
Wingate, C. F. (1993). Exploring the karate way of life: Coping,
commitment, and psychological well-being among traditional karate
participants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Temple University,
Philadelphia.